Touch, like the other senses, has many facets. In this chapter, we consider touch very broadly, so that it includes not just dogs’ physical contact with the world but also their interactions with their physical environment and with other dogs and people.
Dogs touch the world, quite literally, when they walk, run, play,
and sniff. Part of our exploration of touch, then, involves physical activity,
such as going on walks, romping around a dog park, and riding in a car. Dogs
touch noses when they say hello, they may touch nose-to-butt to gather
information about one another, and they touch us when they rub against our legs
or curl up next to us in bed. And, of course, we touch them when we pet, groom,
and hug our canine friends.
We know less about the canine sensory experience of touch than
we do about their sense of smell or taste. We know little, for example, about
how dogs perceive human touch and why some dogs seem to like being touched
while others don’t. Does an aversion to touch develop during the socialization
process, and what kinds of early experiences might lead dogs to feel uncomfortable
rather than soothed by human touch? Why do some dogs simply seem to dislike
human hands? In cases where a dog has an aversion to being touched, this needs
to be honored, and we should always touch dogs on their terms,
not ours. As with human-human touching, consent is important.
Touching often accompanies close encounters between and
among dogs, and it’s possible that it can add or detract from the messages that
are being shared. We’ve seen a dog slowly walk over to a stressed dog, lie down
next to her, and lay a paw over her back as if saying something like “all’s
well” or “I’m here, so relax.” On occasion, dogs will groom one another, and
often they sleep belly to back, feeling comfortable as they spoon. Touching can
also lead to potentially explosive encounters, such as when, for example, one
dog roughly puts his feet on the back of another and gets a quick and forceful rebuke.
If you watch dogs playing at the park, you can see just how unique each dog is
in how they touch other dogs, other people (friends and strangers), and their
surroundings.
COLLARS AND LEASHES: THE BALANCE BETWEEN CONTROL AND FREEDOM
We mediate and control access to the physical and social worlds
of our dogs quite a bit. We do this by deciding when, where, and for how long
dogs get to be outside each day and, perhaps more subtly, by imposing the physical
constraints of collars and leashes, which guide the speed and direction of a dog’s
movements. These tools of control are often necessary, but we should remain
alert to the diverse ways in which they can inhibit a dog’s freedoms and the
ways these devices can themselves be harmful. Our goal should be to use these
tools to facilitate access to a wide variety of positive physical and social experiences
and to allow our dogs as much agency as possible.
Let’s consider collars first, as they make direct contact
with a dog’s neck. Many different types are available, and the type of collar
makes a difference to a dog. Flat collars are by far the most common. They are
what most dogs wear around their necks with ID tags attached. Flat collars can
be okay for walking with dogs who never pull and don’t unexpectedly try to chase
or bolt when on leash. But it is a rare dog who never jerks or pulls. A dog’s
neck is delicate and can be injured by violent jerks on a neck collar and even,
presumably, by sustained hard pulling. Most of us have seen dogs straining so hard
against their collar in their excitement to move forward that they can hardly
breathe and sound a bit like Darth Vader. For this reason, more and more
trainers and veterinarians are recommending that a dog be walked or run on a
chest harness.
Choke collars and collars with sharp prongs, which are designed
to make pulling painful, can also do serious damage if not used with extreme
caution and under carefully controlled conditions. Many trainers advise against
these types of collars altogether, in part because the risk of injury to dogs is
significant. Contrary to popular belief, dogs don’t have really thick skin on
their necks, nor does their fur protect them from pressure on the neck. The San
Francisco SPCA’s website points out that the skin on a human’s neck is ten to
fifteen cells thick, whereas the skin on a dog’s neck is only three to five
cells thick. “So,” they write, “if you think wearing a prong collar would hurt,
imagine how your dog feels.”1 Along
these lines, Dr. Zazie Todd, who runs the Companion Animal Psychology website,
notes, “We tend to think that since dogs have fur they must be more protected
from these things than us with just our skin. But a dog’s neck is a very
sensitive area. If you think about the anatomy of the neck, it contains
essential things like the windpipe. Applying pressure to the windpipe is not
good for any dog, but can be especially serious in brachycephalic dogs that
already struggle to breathe.”2 Prong
and choke collars are typically placed on dogs with serious pulling issues.
Many dogs will still pull, despite the discomfort, and are at risk of injury to
their necks. Chest harnesses that clip in the front are thought to be a better
choice for hard-pulling dogs, since dogs generally dislike the sensation of
being pulled to one side.
Shock collars that allow a human to administer an electric shock
to a dog’s neck from a remote controller are widely considered problematic and
are coming under increasing scrutiny by veterinarians, animal protection
activists, and trainers. Especially as “e-collars” become increasingly cheaper and
more available in pet stores and online, the concern is that dog owners will
use these collars without sufficient background in dog training techniques or
dog behavior, and so these collars will be used in ill-advised and harmful
ways. In the hands of novice users, these collars are very bad news for dogs,
though arguably, shock collars are bad news for dogs no matter what. In
February 2018,
Scotland announced a ban on the use of electric shock collars, citing concerns
about the wide availability of these devices and the growing consensus that use
of these collars is both ineffective and cruel.3
Scotland joins a growing list of countries — which includes Germany, Norway,
Sweden, Austria, Slovenia, Switzerland, Wales, and some states and territories
in Australia — where the use of shock collars has been banned. We hope more
will follow.
Collars and leashes are usually used together, since most collars
can be attached to some sort of leash. One of the compromises dogs make to live
in human environments centers on this tether, and to one degree or another, the
leash will probably remain an enduring aspect of the human-canine relationship.
In fact, leashes may have always been part of this relationship, though we may
wistfully long for a previous time when dogs were allowed more freedom to run,
and leash laws and ordinances were a thing of the future. In one of the
earliest cave engravings depicting dogs, found by archaeologists in a sandstone
cliff in the Arabian desert and likely dating back about eight thousand years,
a hunter with thirteen dogs holds his bow drawn and ready. Two of the dogs in
this engraved image have a thin line running from their neck to the waist of
the hunter, representing what appear to be leashes and suggesting that humans
trained hunting dogs much earlier than previously thought. Archaeologists aren’t
sure whether the thin lines depict an actual rope or leash or are merely symbolic
of the bond between hunter and dog.4
Ultimately, a leash is simply a tool, a kind of umbilical cord
between human and dog that can be used well or poorly. Used well, it gives dogs
access to their world and can be a critically important freedom enhancer.
Without leashes, dogs wouldn’t be able to go many places with us. Used poorly,
the leash can become a source of severe physical and sensory deprivation and
harm. We need to be responsive to what’s happening on both ends of the leash,
and a walk should involve ongoing negotiations and mutual tolerance between dog
and human. Leash pulling is certainly one of the most frequent points of
contention between human and dog, and it may be one reason that many dogs don’t
get walked: It can become a real headache for someone to try to walk a dog who constantly
tugs and pulls and strains. Walking on a leash is not a natural behavior for a
dog, and indeed it goes against their natural instincts to run and explore.
This is why we often need to devote considerable time and attention to training
dogs how to walk nicely when on lead. This leash training is extremely important
for puppies, but even adult dogs who haven’t been properly schooled in polite
leash walking can learn to accommodate their human. For their part, humans will
be well rewarded for time spent helping their dog understand the how and why of
leashes with many happy miles of walking together as a team.
WALKING THE DOG: ON EXERCISE, SHARED TIME, AND POWER STRUGGLES
For humans who live with dogs, walking is both good exercise
and a good way to develop and maintain strong social bonds with their canine
companion. Yet it can also become a power struggle with negative consequences
for both.5 When such a struggle ensues,
it’s usually the dog who gets the short end of the leash.
People often want to know how much physical and sensory exercise
a dog needs each day, as if one could write a prescription: “Walk dog 30
minutes a day, morning and evening, x 7.” While it’s a good question,
unfortunately there’s no gold standard for how much walking a dog needs because
this will be different for every dog and at every life stage. Puppies typically
need a lot of play and exercise time, but they shouldn’t be walked or run
excessively, since their muscles, tendons, and bones are developing. Older dogs
still need to stay active, and it’s extremely important for them to get exercise
that’s appropriate for them. Of course, as they age, some dogs may need shorter
and easier walks, with perhaps even more time for sniffing, and it’s
wrong-headed to assume that older dogs don’t have much zest for life or don’t
need any walks.
Obviously, dogs need physical exercise, but there are
limits. Yes, too much of a good thing can be harmful. We both live in Boulder
County, an area with an extremely high census of superathletes. You can see
their wiry, sweaty bodies rolling into the coffee shops on weekend mornings,
just having completed a swim or a bike ride, followed by a ten- or fifteenmile trail
run with their dog. Dogs love to run, and some might enjoy a fifteen-mile run,
but our canine companions want to make us happy and will often continue running
or hiking well beyond what is comfortable for them. We need to set safe limits
and pay close attention to what works for them. There’s no shame if a dog balks
at a long hike or run and simply wants to rest. If a dog with their behavior
tells us, “Honey, not today. I’m tired,” we should respect this request.
Notwithstanding the overexercised dogs of Boulder’s
superathletes, one generalization is safe: Most pet dogs don’t get enough
exercise and don’t get to spend adequate time outside of their homes and yards
exploring the world. A common figure thrown around by dog trainers is that an
hour of exercise in the morning and an hour in the afternoon or evening is a
good goal. Yet very few dogs are lucky to get this much walking or running
time. A recent survey of dog owners in the United Kingdom, for example, found
that, on average every day, 20 percent of dogs are walked
for an hour, 43 percent of dogs are walked for thirty-one to
fifty-nine minutes, 34 percent are walked for eleven to thirty minutes, and
3
percent are walked ten minutes or less. These percentages only refer to
dogs who actually get walked. Believe it or not, the survey found that about
ninety-three thousand dogs in Britain are never walked at all, ever.6
However, for many people, sharing your life with a dog means
taking some sort of daily walk, and this often becomes a set routine: same
time, same place, same route. That said, despite this sense of routine, the
walk itself can be many things, and each day it can be something different. There’s
always far more going on than simply snapping on a leash and heading out the
door. Sometimes we may treat the dog walk as a necessary chore, and sometimes
as a chance to get some exercise ourselves. Sometimes we hurry our dog to do their
business quickly, and sometimes we let them linger. Sometimes we may walk with
our dog, treating the activity as sacred time together and a chance
to enjoy each other while enjoying nature. Other times we may only walk for
our dog, letting our dog do whatever they want while we daydream, send
texts, talk to friends, and are mentally elsewhere.
How we view a dog walk may suggest certain things about how
we perceive our relationship with our dog. For instance, consider for a moment
these questions: Who is the walk for? Is it for our daily exercise or the dog’s?
And what is the walk for? Is it intended to get somewhere, to get out into
nature, to poop, to let the dog experience dogness by sniffing wherever they
choose? Whatever our perspective, the walk is an arena where the power
relations between a dog and their human companion are negotiated. As an
example, the tightness of a dog’s leash during a walk may tell us something
about that particular human-dog relationship in that moment. A slack leash may
indicate that human and dog are walking in harmony, whereas a tight leash may
suggest conflicting “agencies,” that is, conflicting ideas about where the walk
should go, how quickly they should be moving, and who is leading the way — in
other words, who’s controlling whom.
Thomas Fletcher and Louise Platt, two researchers working in
the field of animal geography, have recently published an interesting study, “(Just)
a Walk with the Dog? Animal Geographies and Negotiating Walking Spaces.”7 They suggest that a dog walk
is far more than it first appears: It’s a complex activity in which the
personalities of both the dog walker and the dog are brought into play, and
where the two are involved in a complex negotiation and even, at times, a power
struggle. Walking, Fletcher and Platt write, is both an expression of the human-animal
bond and a key activity through which that bond can be either strengthened or
potentially weakened.
For their research, Fletcher and Platt conducted in-depth interviews
with people in northern England who regularly walk dogs. Most of the dog walkers
they interviewed felt a strong commitment to “listen” to their dogs, and they
thought that the walk was an opportunity to allow dogs a degree of agency and freedom.
Dogs were seen not as objects to be moved around by their owners but as agents
and companions in the walk. The walk, including its timing, length, and
location, was chosen based on what the respondents felt best suited the needs
of the dog. Most of the respondents spoke of the walk as essential for their
dog’s health and well-being, and they believed that two walks a day of thirty
minutes were sufficient. Although most respondents spoke of the walk as
something they were obligated to provide as a part of responsible caregiving,
they also viewed walking with their dog as something they wanted to do and
enjoyed. As Fletcher and Platt note, this contrasts with the general tone of
the literature on dog care, which tends to frame dog walking as an unpleasant
chore.
Fletcher and Platt also found that people perceive their dogs
to have subjective experiences, to feel emotions, and that the walk is about
making dogs happy. “There was widespread belief,” they write, “that dogs are
happiest when out in the open, and it is here that they are able to best
demonstrate their ‘dogness.’” For example, dog owner Jane spoke about walking
her dog, Copper:
One of the biggest joys for us is when one of us stands at one part of the field and the other, and he just runs. And we’ve managed to time him. He does thirty miles an hour. And he looks like a cheetah, he looks like a wild animal. And it just makes your heart, I mean, I feel a physical change in my body when I watch him run, which has never been created by anything else, really.
Again and again in their interviews, Fletcher and Platt found
people referring to the individual characteristics of their dog, listening to
their dog’s unique preferences, and expressing a commitment to making space for
their animal’s agency. This is the ideal we suggest striving for: Treat the
walk as a way to help dogs be dogs within the constraints of human environments
— to take them to wild places and to give them space to run, sniff, chase,
roll, mark, and interact with other dogs and people (or not, as they prefer).
However, just as a walk can be a way for dog and human to
share experiences and strengthen their bond, a walk can also be a time of
anxiety, stress, power struggles, and unpleasant interaction. Stress can arise
between dog and human, which can be expressed through what we might perceive as
“bad behavior” on the part of the dog: lunging at other dogs or at people,
barking and growling, acting distressed or obnoxious, pulling hard on the
leash. When a walk becomes a power struggle, with the dog pulling one way and
the human pulling the other, no one really enjoys the experience.
When this happens, our advice is to ease up on the leash and
negotiate a peace settlement. Recognize that there may be conflicting agendas,
and take some time to reflect on what you want from the walk versus what your
dog, through their behavior, is telling you they want. When there is regular
conflict, before you head out the door, try to adjust your goals and
expectations so that the walk satisfies both of your needs as much as possible.
This will help make sure that walking the dog strengthens your bond and gives
you both the most enjoyment possible.
UNLEASH YOUR DOG: GIVE AMPLE OFF-LEASH TIME
While leashes can be a meaningful symbol of the human-canine
connection — both tethered together, body and soul — leashes are also, in their
literal form, one of the most important constraints we place on our dogs’
freedom. The leash restricts a dog’s movement and reach, their pace and speed, to
what their human prefers or demands. There are places we don’t want dogs to go,
things we don’t want them to touch or bite, and places they shouldn’t dig. The
leash is also meant to restrict a dog’s ability to engage in social
interactions, whether with other dogs or people. We don’t want to allow dogs
free access to others when that encounter might be unwelcome, inconvenient, or
otherwise undesired and unasked for.
Thus, off-leash time provides dogs with a rare opportunity —
the freedom to explore the world at will, physically, mentally, and socially.
We recommend trying to find places where your dog can run free every day on
their own terms. If this isn’t possible, make certain days their
days and do your best to accommodate their needs.
A prerequisite of giving a dog off-leash time is adequate and
appropriate training. Dogs need to know when and how to return to their human
to be safely allowed off leash; in some cases, this is a legal requirement.
Many of the hiking areas in Boulder County, for example, have “voice-and-sight”
regulations: Your dog must come when you call and must always remain within
your sight, or else you can be charged a fine and will be given a stern lecture
by a ranger. The purpose of voice-and-sight regulations is to balance and
accommodate the needs and well-being of everyone who shares the mountains:
wildlife, Boulder residents, and other dogs. Despite the fantastic off-leash
possibilities near Boulder, we see many dogs who are never unclipped. One of
the most common things we both hear is, “Oh, Buddy would love to be able to run
free, but I can’t let him. He won’t come back.” A dog’s freedom is directly
related to how much time and energy their human is willing to devote to
training. It’s unfortunate when a dog is denied the opportunity to be off leash
simply because their human is unwilling to dedicate this time.
That said, it’s also the case that some dogs simply have difficulty
with recall, no matter how much their human guardians work to improve their
recall skills. Some dogs have a very strong prey drive, some have difficulty
focusing attention, and some are just downright stubborn (often like their
humans). For these dogs, a leash may always be necessary when out in the world.
When this is the case, and in general, consider other options to allow
off-leash freedom, as well as to increase a dog’s range of choices and thus
their own sense of self-determination. For instance, if you have a fenced yard,
consider installing a dog door. This is a great enhancement that expands a dog’s
environmental milieu. They can choose to relieve themselves on their own schedule
instead of having to wait to be let out, and they can be outside, breathe fresh
air, and watch and listen for other animals.
Another opportunity for off-leash time, of course, is the dog
park. Dog parks can be great places for dogs. Dogs can run free, interact with
other dogs and humans, and play. Dog parks are the fastest-growing part of city
parks.8 In 2010, there were
569
off-leash dog parks in the hundred largest US cities, a 34
percent jump in five years, while overall parks increased only 3 percent.9 Some dog parks also are making
accommodations for special-needs individuals, and some cities are offering
places for dogs and humans to interact that are in between homes and dog parks.10
Like so many other things, the appropriateness of a dog park
depends on the dog, the park, the humans who frequent it, and all of these
combined. Dog parks can be very stressful for some dogs, and each dog park
develops its own personality, one defined by its particular population, much
like human neighborhoods. Plus, some dogs find dog parks overstimulating or
scary, so listen to your dog. Bella doesn’t really like the dog park unless it
is empty, in which case she enjoys sniffing here and there. Jethro loved dog
parks and loved everyone he met there, human and nonhuman. Maya likes to go
into the park, but there is one dog she dislikes in her neighborhood and who
shares her feelings. They have come very close to getting in a fight, so
Jessica doesn’t take Maya into the park if “the enemy” is there. As with so
many other topics, listen to and get to know your dog, and then honor their
likes, dislikes, and choices.
Dogs are highly social, group-living animals, yet they’re rarely
allowed to engage in group activities (like play) that aren’t contrived and
controlled, since their social interactions are often constrained by leashes,
fences, and humans. As such, it’s difficult to know the importance of giving
individual dogs the opportunity to interact with groups of other dogs,
particularly groups that are stable over time.
At least at dog parks, dogs get to interact with groups of dogs,
though often these are not stable groups, since on any given day, you don’t
know who will show up or if new dogs might arrive. Indeed, sometimes people are
hesitant to allow their dog to romp and zoom around with dogs they don’t know.
However, dogs can learn lessons about social etiquette by interacting with
strange dogs, just like we can. When these sorts of interactions aren’t
allowed, a dog might miss out on meeting some important behavioral needs. More
research in this area would surely be of interest, and it could have important practical
applications that could benefit dog-dog and dog-human relationships.
An example of research that bears on the social dynamics of
groups of unleashed dogs is a study conducted by Zsuzza Akos and her
colleagues, who observed a group of six dogs belonging to the same household as
they went on unleashed walks with their owner. The question the researchers
wanted to explore was how a group of dogs decide the direction of their
collective movements: Is there a leader, and how is this leader chosen, or is
decision-making egalitarian? Researchers watched “leader-follower” relations
and decision-making and tried to determine how each dog’s position in a social hierarchy
was based on developed social networks and personality differences in the dogs.
These social interactions took place during off-leash walking, and the study
would not have been possible, or would have been significantly influenced, by the
presence of leashes. The researchers write, “Groups that are not able to
coordinate their actions and cannot reach a consensus on important events, such
as where to go, will destabilise, and individuals will lose the benefits of
being part of a group.”11 Because
leashes significantly impact group dynamics, further research involving groups
of off-leash dogs (outside of dog parks) may help us better understand canine social
dynamics.
Finally, we feel there is a desperate need for more research
on the topics of leashes, off-leash time, and dog walking. We know that leashes
influence dog-human and dog-dog social dynamics. But we don’t know all that
much about how and why. For instance, many people report that their otherwise amiable
dog gets aggressive or unfriendly toward other dogs when on a leash. Several
books have been written on what is called “leash reactivity,” and leash
reactivity has even become a category of behavioral diagnosis for dogs. But we
don’t have a good handle on why leashes trigger this behavior in some dogs and
not others, nor about a whole range of other possible ways in which being
tethered influences how dogs feel and how they behave.
NURTURE YOUR DOG’S FRIENDSHIPS
One reason dogs like to go to dog parks is to touch, see,
and sniff friends or to meet other dogs who might become future friends. This
is a wonderful enhancement we can offer them. One activity that dogs love is
play. In addition to freedom, play requires two other important ingredients,
namely, fun and friends. And dog parks are a good place for dogs to have all
three. Dogs show preferences for certain individuals. Marc knew two dogs —
Sadie, a small hairy mix of lots of different genes, and Roxy, a lean boxer mix
— who were best friends. When Sadie arrived at the dog park, she immediately peed
and then checked out who was there by lifting her head and sniffing, and then
almost invariably she ran back to the entrance to wait for Roxy, who, if she
was already at the dog park, raced up to Sadie around 95 percent of
the time (according to Roxy and Sadie’s humans). Then they would play as if
they were the only two dogs in the world.
However, an interesting thing happened on the days when Roxy
didn’t show. Sadie would pace along the fence line and look around, clearly
wondering where Roxy was, even as other dogs came up to say hello and ask her
to play. Sadie usually paced for around twenty seconds or so, which is all the
time she needed to establish that Roxy was absent. At that point, Sadie went
off and found other dogs to play with.
How did Sadie know so quickly that Roxy wasn’t there? We
have no idea, but when Sadie chose to give up waiting and go find other friends
with whom to romp, she was correct 99 percent of the time; Roxy
wasn’t coming. Is it safe to say that Sadie and Roxy were friends, and that
they preferred to hang out and play together? Yes, it is, and their humans agreed.
Using her senses, and perhaps even a sense of time, Sadie displayed an uncanny
knack for identifying Roxy’s presence or absence. And if Roxy was missing, did
Sadie ever let her freedom in the dog park go to waste? Never. What dog would
ever do that? She simply went off to find other dogs, familiar or unfamiliar,
with whom to romp.
Playing with friends is a lot of fun because friends know one
another’s personality and play style and can jump right in without having to
formally ask if they want to play or to negotiate the details. When old friends
aren’t present, dog parks allow dogs to make new friends and expand their
social circle. A few years ago, Marc was thrilled when Alexandra Weber, an
eighth grader at a Boulder middle school, emailed him to ask if he would help
her with a science fair project on play in dogs. After enlisting her mother,
Lisa, and her younger sister, Sophia, to become her field assistants, Alexandra
and Marc decided to focus on the question of whether familiar dogs play
differently than unfamiliar dogs. Alexandra thought that simple question had
been studied extensively, but it hasn’t. There are tidbits of ideas scattered
about in the research, but no one has really studied this question in depth.
Concerning this topic, dog trainer Patricia McConnell had previously written, “My
observations suggest that dogs who are less familiar tend to play bow more to
each other than familiar dogs do.”12 This
might be because they must tell the unfamiliar dog what they want to do, since
they don’t know one another’s style of playing and it could be risky to jump
right in. However, we don’t know if that really is so.
In Alexandra’s study, which is an excellent example of
citizen science, she used her two dogs — Tinkerbell, a highly social dog who
loves to play with any dog, and Huggins, who is pickier about his playmates —
as confederates in her study, which she conducted at a local dog park in
Boulder. Alexandra discovered that play was more rough-and-tumble when familiar
dogs played. When they knew the dog with whom they were playing, dogs weren’t
as worried about formalities, and they jumped right into play. All dogs in the
study showed similar behavior. Overall, dogs who knew one another played more
roughly and didn’t take the time to sniff and greet one another. Dogs who didn’t
know one another were more formal and respectful. They took the time to get to
know one another before starting to play by sniffing and nose bumping more than
they did with familiar playmates.
Obviously, this question needs further research, but Marc was
proud that Alexandra and her family became ethologists to help answer it, and
her father also became much more interested in dogs. To top off her efforts,
Alexandra won a science fair award for her research.
KNOW YOUR DOG’S PETTING PREFERENCES
You may not sit around thinking about what it means to have a
pet dog. But the origin of the term pet captures something important
about the human relationship to companion animals. The word pet,
first recorded in 1508, is from the Middle English pety,
meaning small. The term has been applied to both nonhuman animals and women and
can mean (as a verb) “to stroke or pat affectionately” or (as a noun) “something
one feels affection for” and “an animal kept in the domestic setting whose
function is personal companionship or entertainment.” Although the word may
have insulting connotations, it also points to one of the positive components
of our relationship with dogs: the physical touch that brings us together and
forms the glue in the human-animal bond.
You don’t have to be a scientist to know that many, if not
most, dogs like to be petted. There is a scientific explanation for the
positive role of touch in our human-animal friendships. The importance of touch
has been well established for decades, beginning with Harry Harlow’s famous (and
famously horrifying) studies on infant rhesus monkeys. Monkeys who were raised
on wire “mothers” suffered deep psychological trauma because of the lack of
maternal touch. Touch is essential to normal mental and emotional development, not
just for human babies but for all mammals (and perhaps other animals as well).
Touch feels good. It can reduce blood pressure, lower cortisol levels, lower
heart rate, and increase levels of oxytocin in humans and dogs alike.
Hairy-skinned mammals (which includes humans and dogs, among
others) have a group of sensory neurons called C-tactile afferents. Gentle stroking
of these neurons stimulates the release of oxytocin, often called the “love
hormone” because it is associated with feelings of trust and affection in mammals.
These neurons don’t respond to rough touching, pinching, or poking. Petting
feels good to both parties, the one who pets and the one being petted. What
quality and quantity of touch feels good varies from one individual to the next,
and different dogs have different thresholds for when and what kind of petting
feels good and for when petting begins to feel invasive and uncomfortable.
Some dogs simply don’t like being touched, while some don’t
like being touched by strangers or certain types of people. Such dogs are often
labeled as “cranky,” “mean,” or “standoffish,” but this isn’t fair. These dogs
may have good reasons for not liking to be touched, such as having had negative
experiences with rough touching or physical punishment, or this may just be who
they are. We should respect their desires, either way.
Sometimes dogs who have always enjoyed physical contact will
withdraw. This can be because they’re in pain and being touched doesn’t feel
good. It can also be a sign of anxiety or stress. A noticeable change in a dog’s
tolerance for touch should prompt a visit to the veterinarian to make sure everything
is okay.
Touch should always happen on the dog’s terms and with the
dog’s consent. We need to practice reading a dog’s body language for cues about
consent and about when, where, and how they want to be touched. For example, if
a dog moves away from you or their body stiffens, this is a good sign they do
not want to be touched. The better we understand the behavioral repertoire of
dogs in general, and our own dog in particular, the more we will be able to
respect their choices. To this end, closely observe some interactions between
your dog and new people (and even make an ethogram) and watch for signals the
dog gives about their interest in meeting and being touched by an unfamiliar
person. What do you see happening to the tail, the ears, the eyes, the facial
expression, and the position of the body?
The language of “personal space” is often used to teach children
about respecting other people and to explain why it is rude and intrusive to
touch other people or even get extremely close without first asking permission.
Personal
space is a term developed by anthropologists, and according to the English
Oxford Living Dictionary, it is defined as the “physical space immediately
surrounding someone, into which encroachment can feel threatening or
uncomfortable.” The study of personal space is called “proxemics,” and
researchers in this area have found that the amygdala is largely responsible for
determining how much personal space an animal needs and why invasion of the
space activates a fear response.13 We
can use the same basic principle to guide interactions with dogs: Don’t touch
without consent, and don’t enter personal space without asking first. What
constitutes “personal space” will vary from one dog to another.
It’s worth noting that personal space is a two-way street,
and dogs don’t always respect our individual human boundaries. For example,
some people are uncomfortable when a dog (especially an unknown dog) jumps on
them or rubs all over their legs, and at times dogs will ignore human signals.
The human-animal bond can suffer when preferences related to personal space
differ, such as when a very touchy-feely dog is adopted by a human who doesn’t
like touch, or when a standoffish dog is adopted by a human who wants constant physical
contact. Keep this in mind when adopting a dog: Consider your own needs for
personal space and those exhibited by the dog. Naturally, some give and take
will always be necessary.
SIGNS OF AFFECTION: HUGGING AND LICKING
Our love for dogs often gets expressed in hugging, and dogs often
return that affection by licking. These two intimate gestures translate our
feelings of emotional closeness into physical closeness. Many dogs are hug
magnets, and children seem particularly drawn to throw their arms around the
furry neck or torso of a dog.
That said, as with petting, remember that some dogs find hugging
uncomfortable, even frightening, and all dogs might have moments when they do
not want to be hugged. Since hugging requires even more closeness than petting,
an unexpected hug might make a dog nervous, and they may respond by snapping or
even biting. Again, hugging is usually okay if it’s done on the dog’s terms,
and the best advice is to err on the side of caution: When or if you’re unsure,
don’t hug. As always, pay close attention to the personality of the dog. Understand
their preferences and signals of consent.
Of course, dogs can’t hug us in the same way, though they
can certainly climb into our laps, lean in, and snuggle close. They also
probably lick as a sign of affection, and being licked vigorously by a loving
dog is a source of (sticky) joy. But here again, not all people like being
licked, and dogs may not always lick to express emotion. It’s also possible a
dog will lick to use taste to get information about what we’ve recently eaten
or who we’ve kissed.
WHISKERS ARE SENSATIONAL
Dog whiskers are undeniably cute, but they’re not simply cosmetic.
Whiskers, or vibrissae (from the Latin vibrare, “to vibrate”) as they
are technically known, are specialized hairs that help mammals interact with
others and with their environment. Whiskers are different from pelage hair.
Pelage is the hairy, woolly, or furry coat of a mammal, which serves its own
important functions: It insulates, conceals, signals, and protects. Whiskers
are longer, thicker, and stiffer, and they are exquisitely sensitive to touch.
Each vibrissae follicle is distinctly represented in the sensory cortex of the
brain, and each follicle has its own blood and nerve supply. In dogs and some
other mammals, vibrissae are localized to the facial region, but some animals
have vibrissae on other parts of their body, such as the forearms. Dogs have
four sets of vibrissae: on their upper lips, their lower lips and chins, above
the eyes, and on their cheeks. Take a moment right now to get a good look at
your dog’s whiskers and find the four different sets.
Vibrissae are an important part of the sensory apparatus for
nearly all mammals, with the notable exception of the naked ape (that’s us).
Because we don’t have whiskers, we may underestimate their importance to other
mammals. These delicate sensory tools help wild mammals sense danger, find
food, and navigate their environment, particularly during times of low light. Whiskers
also pay a role in the social behavior of some species, such as rats. We don’t
know how whiskers function in the social lives of dogs, but it is likely that
they play some role.
Research conducted in the 1970s on tactile sensation in rats,
mice, and cats established the central role of whiskers. One classic study, for
instance, involved removing the whiskers of rats and measuring performance in a
maze test. Not surprisingly, rats deprived of whiskers had more difficulty completing
a maze than rats with intact vibrissae. In fact, the deprivation of whiskers
was found to be more significant than the deprivation of smell, hearing, or
vision, which also makes clear just how cruel these vibrissae-removal
experiments were. Although whiskers seem to be less critical to dogs than to
rats, they’re still an important part of a dog’s sensory interaction with the
world.
Dog groomers will often trim whiskers, if not specifically asked
to leave them intact. Dogs used for show will often have their whiskers trimmed
to achieve “clean lines” on the face, but the American Kennel Club discourages
whisker trimming in most breeds. They recognize that whiskers serve important functions
and, as in the case of the Pekingese, “add to the desired expression.”14 Because vibrissae are made of
keratin, like pelagic (dense) hair, cutting them isn’t physically painful to a
dog, though plucking them is. Nonetheless, trimming the whiskers removes or
blunts an important sensory modality for dogs. Hopefully, breed and grooming
standards will continue to evolve to embrace the beauty and functionality of dog
whiskers.
DOGS DIG TOGETHER TIME
By “together time,” we mean the quality time that you and your
dog spend together each day. Together time isn’t just about petting your dog,
but it refers more generally to all forms of social, emotional, and physical
closeness. “Together time” is one of the most important enhancements in our book
because what dogs most need from us is us, and that’s the one thing
many dogs don’t get enough of. As with children, dogs enjoy new toys and
special treats and a fancy new bed, but these aren’t what they really care
about. What they really crave is the company of their human companions.
Dogs are social animals, and they form strong attachments to
their humans. Indeed, selection pressures on domestic dogs have favored the “hypersociability”
gene. Dogs don’t just tolerate human presence; they actively seek it out. It’s
safe to say that companion dogs need social closeness with humans,
and deprivation of this social contact poses wellbeing concerns.
The UK government recently appointed a Minister of Loneliness
to address the growing crisis of loneliness among the human population of the
island nation.15 We could very well also use a
Minister of Loneliness for the pet dogs in the world. While Britain has data on
the scope of its human loneliness problem — more than nine million people often
or always feel lonely, according to studies done by the health ministry — we
don’t have good research on the problem of loneliness in companion dogs. But
more and more veterinarians and trainers are starting to talk about canine
welfare problems associated with social isolation and are connecting the dots
between loneliness and growing numbers of behavioral and psychological
disorders among pet dogs.
Millions of dogs are left home alone for long periods of time.
Some are lucky enough to have access to the full house and a fenced yard
through a dog door. Many others are locked in a kennel or basement or bathroom
while their owner is gone, presumably to protect the house from destructive
behaviors. A survey of UK dog owners found that more than a quarter of dog
owners believe it is okay to leave a dog at home for over five hours a day.16 Many dogs are stressed when left
alone. Levels of cortisol in the blood increase, and they sometimes spike the
entire time the dog is alone. Dogs may resort to obsessive barking or to
destructive behaviors such as digging the carpet, chewing the couch, or
destuffing all the pillows on the bed, the very behaviors that can lead owners
to lock a dog in a kennel or garage, which further fuels a dog’s anxiety.
How much alone time is okay for a dog? Nobody really knows,
and surely it depends on the individual dog. Although behaviorists and
veterinarians don’t agree on a single figure, there is loose consensus that
about four hours alone is a comfortable range for an adult dog. Puppies should
only be left for shorter periods, and not longer than they can hold their bladder.
Many people wonder whether dogs know the difference between being left alone
for ten minutes and four hours, since dogs don’t exactly watch the hands of the
clock turn. Anecdotally, people report that their dogs seem more excited and
offer a more enthusiastic greeting after a longer absence. Backing this up is a
study conducted by Therese Rehn and Linda Keeling, in which they observed a
small group of privately owned dogs and videotaped what the dogs did before, during,
and after a thirty-minute, two-hour, and four-hour separation. They found that
dogs tended to offer more intense greeting behaviors, with a higher frequency
of physical activity and attention behavior, after the longer separations. As
they note, their study doesn’t confirm whether dogs distinguish between thirty
minutes and four hours, but it confirms that dogs are affected by the duration
of their time alone.17
The surest cure for loneliness and social isolation is, of course,
togetherness. To increase this, look for ways to include your dog in your
activities — such as bringing your dog along when running errands or attending
a child’s soccer game — and build the structure of each day so that periods of isolation
are broken up by together time. People sometimes complain that their lives are
ruled by their dog — it’s harder to go on trips, make social plans, or even get
necessary work done because they have to be home to “let the dog out.” Exactly.
That’s the reality of living with a dog, and that’s why dog ownership is not
right for everybody.
We also must think about the signals we send our dog. A dog
trainer friend of Jessica’s told her about a family who had hired him to help
with a behaviorally challenged dog. The dog would cry and scratch in his
downstairs kennel all night, while the family was upstairs trying to sleep
through the disturbance. The trainer observed that the family and the dog spent
the evenings before bed together in the living room and kitchen, playing games
and watching TV. The dog was very much part of the activity and given a lot of
attention. As the family got ready for bed, they gave the dog extra love and
petting, and then they escorted him to the kennel, where he would spend the
night separate from everyone else. The dog was being given very mixed signals;
he was part of the family pack until, suddenly, he wasn’t. In this case, the
trainer recommended letting the dog sleep upstairs with the family. They did,
and the dog was much happier, and everyone was finally able to get a good night’s
sleep.
Sometimes people try to address the problem of a lonely dog
by bringing home a second dog so that the dogs have company even when their
human is gone. This can be helpful for many dogs, and in the best cases, two or
more dogs living in the same home will become best friends and add much enjoyment
and enrichment to one another’s lives. But adding a second dog is not a
surefire cure for loneliness. The social bond between dog and human is unique;
the human fills a role that cannot be filled by another dog. Studies suggest,
for example, that separation anxiety is generally no better in dogs left with
another dog than in those left alone. Other research shows that human social
contact provides more comfort to a socially isolated dog than one of the dog’s
own siblings.18 So, if you are gone for long
hours each day, adding a second dog to your home may simply result in having
two lonely dogs rather than one. Furthermore, multiple-dog households have their
own challenges. A new dog will change the social dynamics of the family, often
in ways that make things more difficult rather than easier. Dog-dog aggression
can be a serious problem and can sometimes lead to untenable situations, where
one dog will wind up needing to be rehomed.
DOGS ALSO NEED ALONE TIME
If you have a dog with separation anxiety, this enhancement might
make you chuckle (or perhaps cry). But just like people, dogs may want to have
some time to themselves. This is particularly the case in homes with children
or with a lot of activity and stimulation. It is important for every dog to have
a “safe zone” — a place the dog can retreat to and be allowed not
to interact or be touched. Mishka, a somewhat zaftig husky with whom
Marc shared his home, loved to nestle in a corner behind a bed when she’d had
enough of her humans. She made it clear what she was doing, and there was only
room for her. When she was ready to interact, she’d have to back out of her husky
cave, which she did with impressive agility. Some people provide their dog with
a crate or kennel — with the door always open — which is designated “dog property”
and off-limits to children and other humans in the house.
by Marc Bekoff and Jessica Pierce.
Note
1.
San Francisco SPCA, “Prong Collar Myths and Facts,” accessed September 7, 2018, https://www.sfspca.org/prong/myths.
2.
Zazie Todd, “What Is Positive Punishment in Dog Training?” Companion
Animal Psychology (blog), October 25, 2017, https://www.companionanimalpsychology.com/2017/10/what-is-positive-punishment-in-dog.html.
3.
Laura Goldman, “Cruel Shock Collars Are Now Banned in Scotland, But Still Not
in the US,” Care2.com,
February 27,
2018,
https://www.care2.com/causes/cruel-shock-collars-now-banned-in-scotland-but-still-not-in-the-us.html.
4.
David Grimm, “These May Be the World’s First Images of Dogs— and They’re
Wearing Leashes,” Science, November 16, 2017, http://www.sciencemag.org/news/2017/11/these-may-be-world-s-first-images-dogs-and-they-re-wearing-leashes.
5.
Jessica Pierce, “Not Just Walking the Dog,” All Dogs Go to Heaven (blog), Psychology
Today, March 16,
2017,
https://www.psychologytoday.com/blog/all-dogs-go-heaven/201703/not-just-walking-the-dog.
6.
PDSA
Animal Wellbeing PAWS Report 2017, page 11, https://www.pdsa.org.uk/media/3290/pdsa-paw-report-2017_online-3.pdf.
7.
Thomas Fletcher and Louise Platt, “(Just) a Walk with the Dog? Animal
Geographies and Negotiating Walking Spaces,” Social and Cultural Geography (2018), https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/full/10.1080/14649365.2016.1274047.
8.
See “Dog Parks Lead Growth in U.S. City Parks,” Trust for Public Land, April 15, 2015, https://www.tpl.org/media-room/dog-parks-lead-growth-us-city-parks;
and “2014 City
Park Facts,” 2014,
Trust for Public Land, https://www.tpl.org/2014-city-park-facts.
This website contains numerous details about many different aspects of urban
parks.
9.
Information on the history of dog parks can be found in Laurel Allen, “Dog
Parks: Benefits and Liabilities,” Master’s capstone project, University of
Pennsylvania, May 29,
2007,
http://repository.upenn.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=1017&context=mes_capstones;
and Haya El Nasser, “Fastest-Growing Urban Parks Are for the Dogs,” USA
Today, December 8, 2011, http://usatoday30.usatoday.com/news/nation/story/2011-12-07/dog-parks/51715340/1.
10.
See Samantha Bartram, “All Dogs Allowed,” Parks and Recreation, National
Recreation and Park Association, January 1, 2014, https://www.nrpa.org/parks-recreation-magazine/2014/january/all-dogs-allowed;
and F. Gaunet, E. Pari-Perrin, and G. Bernardin,
“Description of Dogs and Owners in Outdoor Built-Up Areas
and Their More-Than-Human Issues,” Environmental Management 54, no. 3 (2014): 383–401, doi: 10.1007/s00267-014-0297-8.
11.
Zsuzsa Akos et al., “Leadership and Path Characteristics during Walks Are Linked
to Dominance Order and Individual Traits in Dogs,” PLOS Computational
Biology 10,
no. 1 (2014): e1003446, https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1003446.
12.
Patricia McConnell, “A New Look at Play Bows,” The Other End of the Leash
(blog), March 28,
2016,
http://www.patriciamcconnell.com/theotherendoftheleash/a-new-look-at-play-bows.
13.
See Edward T. Hall, The Hidden Dimension (New York:
Anchor Books, 1986),
which is a classic work on personal space and proxemics.
14.
See “Pekingese,” CyberPet, accessed September 8, 2018, http://www.cyberpet.com/dogs/pekingese.htm.
15.
Ceylan Yeginsu, “U.K. Appoints a Minister for Loneliness” New
York Times, January 17, 2018, https://www.nytimes.com/2018/01/17/world/europe/uk-britain-loneliness.html.
16.
“Number of Hours Pet Dogs Left Alone in the House in the United Kingdom (UK) in
2013,”
Statista, https://www.statista.com/statistics/299859/dogs-hours-left-alone-in-the-united-kingdom-uk.
17.
Therese Rehn and Linda J. Keeling, “The Effect of Time Left Alone at Home on
Dog Welfare,” Applied Animal Behaviour Science 129 (2011): 129–35.
18.
David S. Tuber et al., “Behavioral and Glucocorticoid Responses of Adult
Domestic Dogs (Canis familiaris) to Companionship and Social
Separation,” Journal of Comparative Psychology 110 (1996): 103–8, https://www.researchgate.net/profile/michael_hennessy5/publication/14352984_behavioral_and_glucocorticoid_responses_of_adult_domestic_dogs_canis_familiaris_to_companionship_and_social_separation.
0 comments:
Post a Comment